TY - JOUR
T1 - Korean putting verbs do not categorize space contrastively in terms of "tightness of fit"
AU - Kawachi, Kazuhiro
N1 - Funding Information:
I would like to express my deepest thanks to Leonard Talmy and Jean-Pierre Koenig for all of the valuable advice and highly insightful comments that they gave me on this study. I would also like to convey my profound gratitude to David Zubin for his constructive criticism. My special thanks go to Melissa Bowerman, Soonja Choi, and Eve Clark for taking time to have discussions with me on this study. My sincere gratitude also goes to Sang-gu Kang and Todd McDaniels for reading an earlier version of this paper and giving me helpful comments. I am greatly indebted to Seungjoon Jun (Seoul), Jeongmee Kim (Seoul), Myoyoung Kim (Seoul), Sang-gu Kang (Busan), Yongeun Lee (Busan), Sunyoung Hong (Taegu), Taeho Kim (Incheon), Heeyoung Choi (Seoul), and other adult native speakers of Korean, who are all in their twenties or thirties at present, for being my Korean language consultants. I would also like to thank anonymous referees for their useful comments. Of course, all errors are mine. Portions of this paper were presented at the 8th International Cognitive Linguistics Conference at University of La Rioja, Spain. The present study was supported partially by the Department of Linguistics and the Center for Cognitive Science at the University at Buffalo.
PY - 2007/10
Y1 - 2007/10
N2 - The present study refutes Choi and Bowerman's (e.g. 1991) analysis of Korean putting verbs as showing a crosslinguistically unusual pattern of categorizing space in terms of "tightness of fit." It shows that, in adult Korean, the differences between the "tight-fit" verb kki-ta ('slide the Figure, which is surrounded by or surrounds the Ground, along the inner or outer side surface(s) of the Ground with friction produced between the side surfaces of the two objects') and two "loose-fit" verbs, neh-ta ('move the Figure to a locus where it is surrounded by the Ground') and noh-ta ('deposit the Figure by releasing one's hold of it and leave it the way it exists'), cannot be reduced to "tightness of fit"; in addition, they even show different syntactic behavior. First, the two types of verbs refer to different components and phases of a motion event. Neh-ta and noh-ta are path verbs that refer to the endpoint of a path, whereas kki-ta is a manner + path verb that focuses on an earlier phase; in particular, kki-ta normally requires the Figure and the Ground to produce friction in the course of the motion. Because of this difference, in a verb compound with the connective -e/-a, in which manner/cause verbs must precede path verbs, kki-ta occupies the manner/cause-verb slot, whereas neh-ta fills the path verb slot; noh-ta is usually used as an aspectual marker in the verb compound and goes into the final slot, after the above two slots. Despite Choi and Bowerman's argument for young children's acquisition of language-specific principles of categorization of the input language, their child subjects' errors on their production of kki-ta show deviations from adult usage toward a much simpler pattern of categorizing putting events in terms of whether or not the Figure can easily be separated from the Ground at the endpoint of the path. The present study points out that the experimental studies conducted in recent years, which seem to show that preverbal infants respond categorically to putting events that appear to contrast in terms of "tightness of fit," allow different interpretations, because the categorization principle that the infants used may not be based on "tightness of fit.".
AB - The present study refutes Choi and Bowerman's (e.g. 1991) analysis of Korean putting verbs as showing a crosslinguistically unusual pattern of categorizing space in terms of "tightness of fit." It shows that, in adult Korean, the differences between the "tight-fit" verb kki-ta ('slide the Figure, which is surrounded by or surrounds the Ground, along the inner or outer side surface(s) of the Ground with friction produced between the side surfaces of the two objects') and two "loose-fit" verbs, neh-ta ('move the Figure to a locus where it is surrounded by the Ground') and noh-ta ('deposit the Figure by releasing one's hold of it and leave it the way it exists'), cannot be reduced to "tightness of fit"; in addition, they even show different syntactic behavior. First, the two types of verbs refer to different components and phases of a motion event. Neh-ta and noh-ta are path verbs that refer to the endpoint of a path, whereas kki-ta is a manner + path verb that focuses on an earlier phase; in particular, kki-ta normally requires the Figure and the Ground to produce friction in the course of the motion. Because of this difference, in a verb compound with the connective -e/-a, in which manner/cause verbs must precede path verbs, kki-ta occupies the manner/cause-verb slot, whereas neh-ta fills the path verb slot; noh-ta is usually used as an aspectual marker in the verb compound and goes into the final slot, after the above two slots. Despite Choi and Bowerman's argument for young children's acquisition of language-specific principles of categorization of the input language, their child subjects' errors on their production of kki-ta show deviations from adult usage toward a much simpler pattern of categorizing putting events in terms of whether or not the Figure can easily be separated from the Ground at the endpoint of the path. The present study points out that the experimental studies conducted in recent years, which seem to show that preverbal infants respond categorically to putting events that appear to contrast in terms of "tightness of fit," allow different interpretations, because the categorization principle that the infants used may not be based on "tightness of fit.".
KW - Korean
KW - Language acquisition theory
KW - Motion verbs
KW - Spatial categorization
KW - Tightness of fit
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=34447631834&partnerID=8YFLogxK
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/citedby.url?scp=34447631834&partnerID=8YFLogxK
U2 - 10.1016/j.lingua.2006.11.003
DO - 10.1016/j.lingua.2006.11.003
M3 - Article
AN - SCOPUS:34447631834
SN - 0024-3841
VL - 117
SP - 1801
EP - 1820
JO - Lingua
JF - Lingua
IS - 10
ER -